The German language is famous for its nominal compounds, not just within Germany but also beyond. Words such as “Donaudampfschifffahrtsgesells
These newly created words are called ad hoc compounds. Some are only rare and new because people hardly ever talk about the subject, such as in “Robbenzucht” (seal breeding). But there are also more creative examples like “Gewürzfischlein” (little salty snacks in the shape of fish) and “Lindwurmfeste-Urgestein” (description of an old dragon in Walter Moers’ Zamonia novels) or even wordplays such as “Magermilchschnitte” (as a creative insult for a professional – and, of course, very thin – model).
Effect on Translation
With regard to the translation of such compounds, there are two main questions: firstly, is there a difference in the frequency of these ad hoc compounds and compounds in general between German original texts and translations into German? And secondly, what do translators into French and Italian make of these typically German structures, and when do translators into German decide to create such a word?
A recent study conducted by the author in her PhD thesis at Germany’s Saarland University tries to provide some answers. The analysis comprises a quantitative as well as a qualitative part, which are presented below in a short overview:
In order to find out about the frequencies of (ad hoc) compounds in German originals and translations, the author compiled a corpus made up of extracts from ten different novels each for German originals, German translations from French and German translations from Italian.
The resulting three subcorpora comprise ca 25,000 words each, which is enough data to obtain reliable results. This quantitative analysis showed that the relation of (all kinds of) nominal compounds in originals and nominal compounds in translations is 1.5:1 while there are even twice as many ad hoc compounds in German originals as compared to translations. Pearson’s chi-squared test was used to assess the significance of the results and showed that these are not random chance but genuine differences between originals and translations.
German Texts Need German Structures
This relative lack of nominal compounds in translations – a very typical structure for genuine German texts – might be one of the reasons why translations tend to be judged unfavorably compared to originals.
For the second part of the study, a total of 448 ad hoc compounds were analysed in detail, i.e. the author chose five novels from each subcorpus and extracted 30 ad hoc compounds each (with one translation from Italian featuring only 28 ad hoc compounds in total). This gives us 150 German original ad hoc compounds with their respective French and Italian translations and another 150/148 German translated ad hoc compounds with their respective French/Italian original.
The identified German compounds were compared with their translations or originals regarding their structure, semantics and creativity. To this end, a specific scheme was elaborated, including a detailed description of different possible kinds of creativity, which might not in all cases conform to an intuitive understanding of “creative”, but which offers a scientific means for working with a concept that very often defies systematic analysis.
Creativity Needed
In a last step of the qualitative analysis, the equivalence of original and translation was judged based on the three levels structure, semantics and creativity – and a total of 42.5% of translations proved to be fully equivalent, which shows that if ad hoc compounds appear in a text, translators know rather well how to deal with them.
Furthermore, translations with semantic changes, though certainly not equivalent on the semantic level, can be both highly creative and fully adequate, as the following example demonstrates:
[…] Eine etwa fünfzigjährige fettleibige Frau von der Statur eines Sumoringers erschien, die einen Yorkshireterrier an der Leine führte. […] Während die Frau ihm weiterhin missbilligende Blicke zuwarf, pinkelte der Bonsaihund an einen Busch.
(Marco Malvaldi (2011): Im Schatten der Pineta, translation by Monika Köpfer, Munich, Piper, S. 79)
La bimba stava per rispondere quando nella piccola radura davanti alla panchina arrivò, annunciata da un rumore di foglie, una cinquantenne grassa come un lottatore di sumo con al guinzaglio uno Yorkshire. […] Intanto la donna continuava a guardarli male, mentre l’acconto di cane si esibiva in una ridicola pisciatina su un cespuglio […].
(Marco Malvaldi (242011 [2007]): La briscola in cinque, Palermo, Sellerio, S. 73)
In this example, the translator was most likely inspired by the concept of “Japanese culture” introduced by the “sumo wrestler”, which she then pursued to replace the “down payment on a dog” with “bonsai dog” – a solution which some native speakers of Italian think is even better and funnier than the original.
A key benefit of the work presented above is its systematic approach to creativity, giving objective criteria for identifying words that are (potentially) creative. Possibly even more important, however, is the fact that the author was able to show that even though word formation is only rarely mentioned in standard literature in translation studies, it is of major importance for (not only literary) translators’ everyday work and thus merits further investigation.
Anne Weber (2016): Von Bonsaihund, Runzelmaulwurf und Monchichi-Bärchen. Eine Studie zu Ad-hoc-Nominalkomposita des Deutschen in der Translation anhand deutscher, französischer und italienischer Ausgangs- und Zieltexte, Saarbrücken, universaar. Available on Open Access
On 27th October Dr. Anne Weber was awarded the Dr.-Eduard-Martin-Preis for outstanding dissertations by the Universitätsgesellschaft des Saarlandes e.V.
Photography of Dr. Anne Weber by Studioline